Early prediction of language and literacy problems: is 18 months too early?

View article
PeerJ
The rate of ATs with SLI in the current study is higher than expected, possibly due to sampling bias, if parents whose children had language problems were more willing to consent to the follow-up. In the original study with a larger sample, the rates were 29% of LTs and 14% of ATs (see Bishop et al., 2012).

Main article text

 

The term ‘late talkers’ (LTs) generally refers to young children aged 18- to 35-months who are slow to develop spoken language in the absence of any known primary cause (Rescorla, 2011). Prevalence rates for LTs differ according to the inclusion criteria and the population sampled, but a recent study of a large community sample suggested that as many as 20% of young children can be classified as LTs (Reilly et al., 2007). A key question is whether LTs should be a cause for concern; here there can be a mismatch between views of academic researchers and those concerned with policy.

Research evidence shows that there is a lack of stability in language across early childhood development—particularly when measured by vocabulary—making prediction of outcomes from infancy unreliable. For example, while the early language difficulties of some LTs persist into childhood, the majority of LTs perform in the average range by pre-school (Rescorla, 2011). Moreover, a significant number of children show late emerging language delay: that is, children who were not originally classified as LTs go on to exhibit language difficulties in the pre-school years (e.g., Dale et al., 2003; Henrichs et al., 2011). It is also important to consider the longer-term outcomes of LTs, given the proposal that children whose language skills appear to have normalised will in fact manifest language and literacy difficulties later in development—so called illusory recovery (Scarborough & Dobrich, 1990; but see Dale et al., 2014). In fact, the majority of LTs perform in the average range on language and literacy measures in the later primary school years and beyond, though often at a level significantly below that of their unaffected peers (e.g., Dale & Hayiou-Thomas, 2013; Paul et al., 1997; Rescorla, 2002; Rescorla, 2005; Rescorla, 2009; Rice, Taylor & Zubrick, 2008). In the main, then, the language difficulties of most LTs are short-lived.

Nevertheless, outside of academia there are those who claim that LTs should be identified and supported early on. The logic is that by intervening early, there is a better chance of avoiding the worst outcomes and of making effective changes while the brain is still plastic. This viewpoint is articulated by the C4EO Early Intervention Expert Group (2010), who note that many children with language delay catch up with their peers, but assert that this is because they have had “the right support.” This is rather misleading, since most studies report good outcomes despite lack of intervention. This is a critical point, because if there is spontaneous improvement in LTs, then early intervention is not warranted, especially if it takes funds from other deserving causes. Unfortunately, spontaneous improvement can also reinforce the misapprehension surrounding outcomes of LTs: if these children are identified and provided with intervention, they then improve, and the improvement is attributed to the intervention. The lack of treatment effect is only evident if one studies an untreated group of LTs, as was done in the study by Wake et al. (2011). Nevertheless, the view remains among some of those influencing policy that a child’s vocabulary level at or before 2 years of age can be used to predict language and pre-literacy skills at school entry (e.g., Roulstone et al., 2011).

An important goal, then, is to be able to distinguish better between early language difficulties that will resolve versus persist – that is, to improve prediction of outcomes at an individual level. This might be achieved by waiting until later in development, once language may have become more stable, or by identifying better predictors early on. Here, we take the former approach. We report a follow-up investigation of a sample of children who were identified as LTs at 18 months old, and of their peers who were classed as average talkers. Our first aim was to compare the language and literacy outcomes of these groups of children. Based on previous findings, we hypothesised that the LTs’ school-age language and literacy skills would be in the average range, but fall significantly below those of their unaffected peers. Information regarding language status at age 4 (typical development vs. specific language impairment) was available for a subsample of children, allowing us to explore a second hypothesis that language status at 4 years would be more indicative of reading and language outcomes than language status at 18 months.

Method

Participants

Measures

Vocabulary knowledge

Phonological short-term memory

Phonological awareness

Reading accuracy

Reading comprehension

Nonverbal ability

Procedure

Results

Discussion

Supplemental Information

Data for full sample (for analyses reported in Tables 1 and 2) and sub sample (for analyses reported in Table 3)

DOI: 10.7717/peerj.1098/supp-1

Additional Information and Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

Author Contributions

Fiona J. Duff conceived and designed the study, collected the data, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Kate Nation conceived and designed the study, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Kim Plunkett provided access to the time 1 data and to norms for OCDI, and reviewed drafts of the paper.

DVM Bishop analyzed the data, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Human Ethics

The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):

Informed parental consent was given for all participants, and ethical approval was granted by the University of Oxford’s Central University Research Ethics Committee, as part of a broader and related research project.

Reference number: MSD/IDREC/C1/2012/56.

Data Deposition

The following information was supplied regarding the deposition of related data:

Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/t35af/.

Funding

This research was funded by the Nuffield Foundation (grant number EDU/40062 awarded to K Nation and K Plunkett). DVM Bishop is supported by the Wellcome Trust Programme grant 082498/Z/07/Z. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

20 Citations 8,227 Views 1,342 Downloads