Effects of elevated CO2 on predator avoidance behaviour by reef fishes is not altered by experimental test water

View article
PeerJ

Introduction

Rising concentrations of atmospheric carbon dioxide (CO2) have caused an increased uptake of CO2 by the ocean, leading to a decline in seawater pH and changes in the relative concentration of carbonate and bicarbonate ions, a process called ocean acidification (Sabine et al., 2004; Doney, 2010). The partial pressure of carbon dioxide (pCO2) at the ocean surface is increasing at the same rate at the atmosphere (Doney, 2010) and thus marine species will need to deal with rising CO2 levels in addition to declining pH and other changes in ocean chemistry (Portner, Langenbuch & Reipschlager, 2004). Recent models suggest an amplification of seasonal cycles in ocean pCO2 as atmospheric CO2 continues to rise, such that surface ocean pCO2 will reach 1,000 μatm in summer for atmospheric CO2 concentrations that exceed 650 parts per million. Hypercapnic conditions (>1,000 μatm CO2) are projected to occur in up to half the surface ocean by 2100 on the current CO2 emissions trajectory (McNeil & Sasse, 2016).

Elevated CO2 levels can have a variety of effects on the physiology, life history and behaviour of marine organisms (Portner, Langenbuch & Reipschlager, 2004). Recent studies have demonstrated that high CO2 levels can fundamentally alter the behaviour of marine fishes and some invertebrates (reviewed by Briffa et al., 2012; Clements & Hunt, 2015; Heuer & Grosell, 2014; Nagelkerken & Munday, 2016). Of particular concern is an impaired response to the threat of predation, because it may increase mortality rates in natural populations (Munday et al., 2010; Ferrari et al., 2011a; Chivers et al., 2014). Larval and juvenile reef fish can exhibit an innate aversion to chemical cues released by predators and chemical alarm cues from injured conspecifics, because they indicate a heightened risk of predation (Holmes & McCormick, 2010; Dixson, Pratchett & Munday, 2012). Individuals typically respond to these chemical cues by reducing activity and seeking shelter (Ferrari, Wisenden & Chivers, 2010). However, larval reef fish that have been reared at near-future CO2 levels do not reduce activity or stop feeding in the presence of chemical alarm cues (Ferrari et al., 2011a; Lönnstedt et al., 2013) and even become attracted to chemical cues from a predator (called predator odour from herein) and chemical alarm cues when presented in a two channel flume (Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Welch et al., 2014). Furthermore, juvenile fish exposed to high CO2 lose their ability for associative learning (Ferrari et al., 2012), a process that enables fine-tuning of risk assessment by the association of chemical alarms cues with the identity of specific predators. Finally, high CO2 affects the kinematics of predator–prey interactions, with juvenile prey exposed to elevated CO2 allowing predators to get closer before responding (Allan et al., 2013) and exhibiting reduced escape speeds and distances compared with fish reared at current-day CO2 levels (Allan et al., 2013; Allan et al., 2014). These changes in behaviour alter the outcome of predator–prey interactions, leading to significantly increased rates of mortality of small juveniles in mesocosm experiments (Ferrari et al., 2011b) and in fish transplanted to natural coral reef habitat (Munday et al., 2010; Munday et al., 2012; Chivers et al., 2014).

Many of the pioneering studies on the effects of elevated CO2 on reef fish behaviour (e.g., Munday et al., 2009; Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Ferrari et al., 2011a; Ferrari et al.,2011b; Cripps, Munday & McCormick, 2011) involved rearing fishes in control and elevated CO2 conditions and then testing their responses to chemical cues in control water only. This method was chosen in these early experiments owing to logistical constraints and because pilot experiments showed that the response of high-CO2 exposed fish to predator cue did not differ if the cue was presented in either control or high CO2 water (Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Munday et al., 2010). Furthermore, studies with freshwater fishes had shown that a pH reduction of 0.5 units in freshwater (<pH 6.5) irreversibly alters the structure of chemical alarm cues and can dramatically affect prey antipredator responses (Brown et al., 2002; Leduc et al., 2004; Leduc, Kelly & Brown, 2004; reviewed in Leduc et al., 2013). Consequently, there was concern that testing marine fish in CO2-acidified water (albeit still above pH 7.5) could confound the interpretation of any effects of high CO2 on the behavioural response of the fish with a diminished efficacy of the chemical cue itself. Testing in control water prevented this potential problem. Finally, small-scale laboratory experiments often have limited ecological relevance and it is challenging to extrapolate their findings to natural communities and ecologically relevant spatial scales (Nagelkerken & Munday, 2016). Consequently, it was critical to test the effects of exposure to high CO2 in natural habitats in the field. This involved transplanting larval fish that had been reared in either control or high CO2 seawater back into natural coral reef habitat and monitoring their behaviour (Munday et al., 2010; Munday et al., 2012; McCormick, Watson & Munday, 2013).

Since these initial studies, a number of other experiments have been conducted in which the respective treatment seawater in which the fish were reared (control or high CO2) has been used in the experimental trials, confirming the results of earlier studies (e.g., Allan et al., 2014; Dixson et al., 2015; Welch et al., 2014; Nagelkerken et al., 2016). Nevertheless, a more thorough validation study of the test water used in previous antipredator experiments is warranted. Here, we compared the use of control versus high CO2 seawater (called test water from hereafter) in experimental trials designed to test the antipredator responses of larval reef fishes reared in high CO2. We conducted antipredator experiments for two species that have been widely used in previous high CO2 experiments, the clownfish Amphiprion percula and the ambon damselfish Pomacentrus amboinensis. Specifically, we tested if: (1) using control or high CO2 water in a two channel flume influenced the response of larval clownfish to predator odour in clownfish reared in either control or high CO2; and (2) using control or high CO2 water in the test arena influenced the kinematic response of larval damselfish to a startle stimulus in fish reared in either control or high CO2. Finally, (3) because the effects of high CO2 on fish behaviour appear to be due to altered function of GABA-A neurotransmitter receptors (Nilsson et al., 2012; Hamilton, Holcombe & Tresguerres, 2014; Heuer & Grosell, 2014) we tested if antipredator behaviours were restored in clownfish treated with a GABA antagonist (gabazine) in high CO2 water. Previous studies have demonstrated that abnormal antipredator behaviour of clownfish and damselfish reared in high CO2 conditions is reversed following treatment with gabazine (Nilsson et al., 2012; Chivers et al., 2014). However, these previous studies involved high CO2 fish that were treated with gabazine in control water and then behaviourally tested in control water. In this experiment we therefore conducted the same gabazine treatments as previously used, except the drug was administered and behaviour was tested in high CO2 water.

Materials and Methods

The experiment with larval clownfish was conducted at James Cook University’s (JCU) experimental aquarium facility in Townsville, Australia, where previous high CO2 experiments with this species have been conducted. The experiment with larval ambon damselfish was conducted at Lizard Island Research Station (LIRS) on the northern Great Barrier Reef, where previous high CO2 experiments with this species have been conducted. Both experiments were conducted during February 2016. Because our goal was to assess the use of control seawater in experimental trials of previous experiments (e.g., Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Munday et al., 2010; Munday et al., 2012) we used the same methods as previous experiments, except that we applied a fully factorial design where fish reared in control and high CO2 conditions were behaviourally tested in both control and high CO2 water. Control water was the ambient in the experimental facility at JCU (496 ± 55 S.D. μatm CO2) and at LIRS where water is pumped directly from the Lizard Island lagoon (464 ± 32 μatm CO2). The high CO2 conditions (1,022 ± 37 and 1,051 ± 18 μatm CO2, respectively) matched projections for CO2 levels in open ocean by the end of this century (McNeil & Sasse, 2016).

Response to predator cue

Two clutches of larval clownfish from different parental pairs maintained in ambient seawater conditions at JCU were reared in control and high CO2 conditions from hatching until settlement (12 days post hatching) using standard practices (Wittenrich, 2007). Briefly, larvae were reared in 100 l incubator tanks supplied with a continuous flow of treatment seawater. Water temperature was maintained at 29°C. Photoperiod was 12 hours light and 12 hours dark. A non-viable microalgae blend (Nano 3600–Reed Mariculture™) was drip-fed into the incubator tanks during daylight hours to reduce light levels and enhance larval feeding during the first four days. Larvae were fed live rotifers (15 per ml) for the first four days and then transitioned onto Artemia (5 per ml). Fish were not fed on the morning of testing. Each clutch was divided at hatching so that half the clutch was reared in control seawater (496 ± 55 S.D. μatm CO2) and half reared in the high CO2 treatment (1,022 ± 37 S.D. μatm CO2) for 12–15 days. Two 10,000 L recirculating aquarium systems supplied seawater to the incubator tanks, one supplied control seawater and the other high CO2 seawater. To achieve the desired CO2 level in the high CO2 treatment, seawater was dosed with CO2 to a set pH in a 3,000 L sump using an Aqua Medic AT Control System (Aqua Medic, Bissendorf, Germany). The pCO2 in rearing tanks was measured by nondispersive infrared (NDIR) following the method described by Hari et al. (2008). Air in a closed loop was circulated by a small pump (1 l min−1 flow rate) through a coil of thin-walled (membrane thickness 0.4 mm, outer ø 3.8 mm) medical grade silicone tubing placed in the tank. CO2 in the closed loop was then measured at 1 min intervals with a Vaisala GMP343 infrared CO2 probe (accuracy ± 5 ppm CO2 + 2% of reading over the range of experimental manipulations). Each reading (N = 9 in control and N = 9 in high CO2) lasted 1 hour to ensure complete equilibration of CO2 between tank seawater and the closed loop of air. The average CO2 for the final ten minutes was calculated for each reading. Due to the short duration of the experiment, and because we were primarily concerned about quantifying pCO2, we did not perform alkalinity titrations to paramterize other components of the carbonate system.

The response of settlement stage clownfish (12–15 days old) to predator cue was tested in a two-channel flume (13 cm × 4 cm internal dimensions to the taper) specifically designed for testing the preferences of larval fish to different chemical cues (Gerlach et al., 2007). Larvae were given the choice between a stream of seawater containing predator cue versus a stream of seawater without the predator cue. Both streams of water were either control or high CO2 test water for a given trial. Water from the respective paired sources was gravity fed into the flume at 100 ml min−1, maintained by a flow meter. Water was exchanged after every fish, with random alternation between the use of control and high CO2 test water in the flume. A dye test was conducted with every water exchange to ensure laminar flow and no mixing of the water streams. For each trial, a single fish was placed in the centre of the downstream end of the flume and allotted a two-minute acclimation period. The position of the fish at 5-second intervals was recorded for the next two minutes. A one-minute rest period followed, during which the water sources were switched to eliminate the effect of any potential side preference by the fish. After the water switch, the fish was re-centred in the downstream end of the flume and the acclimation (2 min) and test period (2 min) were repeated with the predator water on the opposite side of the flume. The time spent in the water stream containing the predator cue was summed for the two 2-minute test periods for each fish. The temperature of the test water was kept within 1°C of the water temperature in the rearing tanks. Three trials where the difference in water temperature between the flume and the rearing tanks exceeded 1°C were discarded to avoid any effect of temperature shock on behaviour. The experimenter was blinded to the rearing treatment of the fish, but it was not logistically possible for the experimenter to also be blinded to the water treatments in the flume.

Predator cues were obtained from the common coral-cod, Cephalopholis miniatus. Five cod collected from the Great Barrier Reef (supplied by Cairns Marine, Cairns QLD) were maintained in 60 L aquaria supplied with a continuous flow of seawater from a different seawater system to that used to rear the clownfish. Each cod was fed one cube of commercial fish food (Marine Food, Fish Fuel Co™) each evening. Water temperature was maintained at 29°C. To obtain predator cues for the experiment, the water was drained from two predator tanks and refilled with water from the same system used to rear the clownfish. To match the methods used in previous experiments, water to the predator tank was turned off and predator cue water was collected after a 2 hour soak time. Predator tanks were aerated to maintain oxygen content and water-bathed to maintain a stable temperature. One of the two predator tanks was dosed with CO2 at the end of the 2 hour soak period, immediately before cue water was extracted. CO2 was dosed at low pressure by hand, with gentle stirring, until the pH matched that of the high CO2 rearing treatment. The other predator tank was also gently stirred but was not dosed with CO2. The individual cod used for predator cues was alternated within and between days. An identical procedure was followed with two tanks that did not house a predator to obtain seawater without predator cue to use in the flume.

Gabazine treatment

The gabazine treatment followed the flume methodology described above, except that fish were treated with 4 mg l−1 gabazine for 30 min immediately before testing in the flume. Eight larval clownfish were randomly selected from the high CO2 treatments and placed in 40 ml of high CO2 water containing gabazine. The fish were gently removed from the gabazine after 30 min and transferred to the flume using a small net. Their response to predator odour was tested in high CO2 water only. Previous experiments demonstrating that gabazine restores antipredator responses in larval fishes reared in high CO2 water have been conducted in control water (Nilsson et al., 2012; Chivers et al., 2014). This experiment only tested if a similar effect was observed when gabazine was administered and fish tested in high CO2 water.

Kinematics of escape response

Settlement stage larval Pomacentrus amboinensis were collected using light traps in the Lizard Island lagoon and transferred to an environmentally-controlled aquarium facility at LIRS. Settlement stage P. amboinensis at Lizard Island have an average age of 15–23 days old (Kerrigan, 1996). Larval fish were assigned randomly to four replicate control (464 ± 32 S.D. μatm CO2) or four replicate high-CO2 (1,051 ± 18 S.D. μatm CO2) aquaria. Twenty five P. amboinensis were housed in each 32 l (38L × 28W × 30H cm) aquarium. Water temperature was maintained at approximately 29.5°C. Fish were kept for 4 days in treatment conditions and fed Artemia sp. twice daily ad libitum. Fish were not fed on the morning of testing. Each aquarium was supplied with control or elevated-CO2 seawater at 750 ml min−1. Elevated-CO2 seawater was achieved by dosing with CO2 to a set pH, following standard techniques (Gattuso et al., 2010). Seawater was pumped from the ocean into 32 l header tanks where it was diffused with ambient air (control) or 100% CO2 to achieve the desired pH (elevated-CO2 treatment). A pH-controller (Aqua Medic, Germany) attached to the CO2 treatment header tank maintained pH at the desired level. Duplicate control and high CO2 systems supplied seawater to two replicate aquaria in each system. The pCO2 in rearing tanks was measured by NDIR as described above (N = 9 in control and N = 5 in high CO2).

After rearing for four days in control or high CO2 conditions, the escape response of the damselfish was tested in both control and high CO2 water (i.e., a fully crossed design). Individual fish were placed into the testing arena (Fig. S1), which consisted of a transparent circular acrylic arena (ø 200 mm) within a larger plastic tank (585 × 420 × 330 mm; 60 L) filled to a depth of 100 mm with either control or high CO2 water. A shallow water depth was used to limit vertical displacement of the fish during escape response trials. Water temperature in the experimental arena was 29°C–30°C and the water treatment used was alternated after every 2nd trial. The arena was illuminated by LED strip lighting (750 lumens) placed above the water surface on the outside of the tank. Five minutes after being released into the testing arena, an escape response was elicited by the release of a tapered metal weight from above the water surface. This was accomplished by turning off an electromagnet to which the metal weight was attached. A length of fishing line was attached to the weight so that it stopped when the tapered tip of the weight only just touched the surface of the water after release from the magnet. In order to provide a sudden stimulation and allow quantification of escape latency, the stimulus was released through a white PVC tube (ø 40 mm, length 550 mm) suspended above the experimental arena, with the bottom edge of the pipe at a distance of 10 mm above the water level. The tube hides the weight from view so there is no visual stimulation before the mechanical stimulation occurs. Fish were startled when they moved to the middle portion of the tank. Escape responses were recorded at 480 frames per second (Casio EX-ZR1000) as a silhouette from below obtained through pointing the camera at a mirror angled at 45°. To minimise visual disturbances, black sheeting surrounded the front of the mirror so that any movement within the room was undetected by the fish. Treatment water in the testing arena was changed every 2nd trial to minimise CO2 loss in the high CO2 treatment water. The rate of CO2 loss from the testing arena was found to be negligible within this time frame. The escape response trials were conducted over two consecutive days. The experimenter was blinded to the rearing treatment of the fish when conducting the trials.

From the videos, we quantified latency to initiate an escape, escape distance, escape speed and maximum speed. A 1 cm line was drawn in the centre of the inner arena to enable distance calibration for video analysis. The moment the stimulus weight hit the water was benchmarked by the first detectable water disturbance in the video. The distance of each fish from the point the stimulus first hit the water was also quantified from the video. All videos were analysed with the observer blinded to the treatments.

Kinematic variables

Kinematic variables associated with the fast-start response were analysed using the image-analysis software Image-J, with a manual tracking plug-in. The centre of mass (CoM) of each fish was tracked for the duration of the response. The following kinematic variables were measured:

(1) Latency to respond (s) was measured as the time interval between the stimulus onset and the first detectable movement leading to the escape of the animal.

(2) Escape distance (m) is a measure of the total distance covered by the fish during the first two flips of the tail (the first two axial bends, i.e., stages 1 and 2 defined based on Domenici & Blake (1997), which is the period considered crucial for avoiding ambush predator attacks (Webb, 1976).

(3) Escape speed (m s−1) was measured as the distance covered within a fixed time (first 24 ms after initial response) which corresponds to the average duration of the first two tail flips (the first two axial bends, i.e., stages 1 and 2 based on (Domenici & Blake, 1997). This period is considered crucial for avoiding predator ambush attacks (Webb, 1976).

(4) Maximum speed (m s−1) was measured as the maximum speed achieved at any time during stage 1 and stage 2.

Data Analysis

Two-way ANOVA was used to compare percent time that larval clownfish spent in predator cue in the flume. Percentage data was logit-transformed for analysis (Warton & Hui, 2011). Treatment water (control or high CO2) and test water (control or high CO2) were the fixed factors. ANOVA was conducted with Statistica (version 13). For the gabazine experiment, the percentage time that larval clownfish treated with gabazine spent in predator cue in the flume was compared with control fish using a t-test on logit-transformed data. Linear mixed effects models (LME) were used to compare escape responses of larval damselfish. Latency, escape distance, escape speed and maximum speed were each tested separately. Treatment water (control or high CO2), test water (control or high CO2) and day (1 or 2) were fixed factors in the model. The distance of each fish from the point the stimulus hit the water was also included as a fixed effect in the model to account for any variation associated with differences in the location of fish in the chamber when the stimulus occurred. Tank was included in each model as a random effect to account for the subsampling of fish from replicate tanks within each CO2 treatment. Best-fit model selection based on AIC values was automated using ‘MuMIn’ (Bartoñ, 2015) within the ‘nlme’ package in the R software package (Pinheiro et al., 2013). The normality, linearity and homoscedasticity of residuals of the models were verified by visual inspection of residual-fit plots. Latency data was log10 transformed before analysis to improve the distribution of the data and a heterogeneous variance structure was used in the model for latency (but not other variables). R code is available in Supplementary Information 1.

This research was conducted in accordance with James Cook University animal ethics guidelines and permits A1961, A1973 and A2080. Fish were collected under permit G12/35117.1 from the Great Barrier Reef Marine Park Authority.

Results

There was a highly significant effect of treatment water on the percentage time that larval clownfish spent in the water stream containing predator odour (F1,57 = 125.91, p < 0.001), but no effect of test water (F1,57 = 0.20, p = 0.66) and no interaction (F1,57 = 0.06, p = 0.81). Laval clownfish reared in control water strongly avoided predator odour, spending <9% of the time in that water stream (Fig. 1). In contrast, larval clownfish reared in high CO2 water were strongly attracted to predator odour, spending nearly 80% of the time in that water stream (Fig. 1).

The effect of rearing treatment water (control or high CO2) and test water used in a two-channel flume (control or high CO2) on the olfactory response of larval A. percula to predator cue.

Figure 1: The effect of rearing treatment water (control or high CO2) and test water used in a two-channel flume (control or high CO2) on the olfactory response of larval A. percula to predator cue.

One stream of water in the flume contained seawater with predator cue and the other stream of water had untreated seawater. Shown is the percent time (mean ± s.e.) that fish from each experimental combination spent in the water stream containing predator cue. The final group shows fish that were treated with 4 mg l−1 gabazine in high CO2 seawater for 30 minutes before testing in the flume. Sample size shown above the bars.

As with the response to predator odour, treatment water had a significant effect on escape responses of larval damselfish, and there was no effect of test water. Distance to stimulus did not affect any of the response variables and was not retained in any of the final models. Latency to respond significantly increased in fish reared in elevated CO2 (F1,6 = 7.76, p = 0.032), with fish displaying latencies that were 30% longer compared with fish reared in control water (Fig.2A). There was no effect of test water or day of testing on latency to respond and these factors were not retained in the final model. Escape distance following stimulation was not significantly different for fish reared in high CO2 compared with control water (F1,5 = 6.15, p = 0.056) although there was a clear tendency for a shorter escape distance in high CO2 fish (Fig. 2B). Day was retained in the final model, but it was not statistically significant (F1,5 = 5.15, p = 0.07). There was no effect of test water on escape distance and it was not retained in the final model. Escape speed and maximum speed responded similarly with both kinematic variables declining significantly in fish reared in high CO2 compared to fish reared in control water (Fig. 2C; F1,5 = 6.81, p = 0.048 and Fig. 2D; F1,6 = 9.016, p = 0.0239). Day was retained in the final model for escape speed, but it was not statistically significant (F1,5 = 3.64, p = 0.115). Day was not retained in the final model for maximum speed. Again, there was no effect of test water for either escape speed of maximum speed and it was not retained in the final model.

The effect of rearing treatment (control or high CO2) and test water used in the experimental trial (control or high CO2) on the escape performance of larval P. amboinensis.

Figure 2: The effect of rearing treatment (control or high CO2) and test water used in the experimental trial (control or high CO2) on the escape performance of larval P. amboinensis.

Variables displayed are: (A) latency, (B) distance, (C) mean speed, and (D) maximum speed. Errors are standard errors. N = 26, 28, 26, 28 for control - control, control - high CO2, high CO2 - high CO2 and high CO2 - control, respectively.

Gabazine reversed the effects of rearing in high CO2 (Fig. 1). Fish reared in high CO2 and then treated with gabazine exhibited a strong avoidance of predator cue, with the response almost identical to fish reared and tested in control water (t22 = 0.04, p = 0.96).

Discussion

Previous studies have demonstrated that CO2 levels projected to occur in the surface ocean by the end of this century (700–1,000 μatm) impair predator avoidance behaviours in larval reef fishes (reviewed in Nagelkerken & Munday, 2016). However, some of these previous studies have tested the behaviour of high-CO2 reared fish using control water in the experimental trials, which could potentially influence the results if there is an immediate physiological response associated with the transfer of fish from high CO2 rearing water to control test water. Here we show that the results of these earlier studies are robust and that antipredator behaviour is not affected by using control water in the experimental trials. Larval fish reared in high CO2 exhibited the same responses to predator odour in a two channel flume and the same kinematic responses to a startle stimulus regardless of whether high CO2 or control water was used in the experimental trial. Similarly, there was no difference in the behavioural response of fish reared in control seawater when tested in either high CO2 or control water. These results demonstrate that the choice of test water has not influenced the results of previous laboratory studies into the effects of high CO2 on antipredator behaviour.

Our results are consistent with preliminary observations carried out in previous studies showing that the test water (control or high CO2) used in a two-channel flume did not alter the behavioural response of larval fishes to predator odour (Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Munday et al., 2010). Furthermore, our study extends these findings to the kinematics of predator avoidance, by showing that the type of water used in the test arena does not affect vital components of the escape response, including latency to respond, escape speed and maximum speed. Together these results show that antipredator behaviours of larval fishes that have been reared in high CO2 are not compromised by a return to control seawater for experimental testing. These findings are also relevant to field-based studies that have tested the effects of high CO2 on reef fishes in their natural habitat (e.g., Munday et al., 2010; Munday et al., 2012; Devine, Munday & Jones, 2012; Devine & Munday, 2013; Ferrari et al., 2011a; McCormick, Watson & Munday, 2013). Such studies are critical for predicting the impacts of high CO2 on reef fish populations; however, they involve rearing larval fish for 4–5 days in high CO2 conditions before transferring them to ambient CO2 conditions in the field. These studies have demonstrated mortality rates from predation are higher in juvenile fish that have been exposed to high CO2 compared with fish exposed to ambient control conditions (Munday et al., 2010; Munday et al., 2012; Ferrari et al., 2011a). Our results indicate that such studies are unlikely to have been affected by any immediate effect on antipredator behaviour caused by transplanting fish from high CO2 conditions in the laboratory to ambient CO2 conditions in their natural habitat. Nevertheless, fish in the field studies also remained in control conditions for a much longer period of time (several days) compared with the laboratory tests conducted here (minutes) and further experiments of longer duration would be required to ascertain any differences in control and high CO2 conditions that might accrue over a longer period of time.

In contrast to the absence of a test water effect on antipredator behaviours, we found that rearing treatment water had a highly significant effect on antipredator behaviour. Consistent with previous studies (Dixson, Munday & Jones, 2010; Munday et al., 2010), larval clownfish reared in high CO2 exhibited a reversal from strongly avoiding predator cue (<10% of time in predator cue in the flume) to a strong attraction to predator cue (nearly 80% of time in predator cue in the flume). Similarly, larval damselfish exposed to high CO2 for 4 days exhibited a slower response to a threat stimulus and a slower escape speed compared with fish kept in ambient control water. There was also a tendency for a shorter distance travelled. These findings are consistent with changes in escape responses that have has been reported previously (Allan et al., 2013; Allan et al., 2014) and provide further confirmatory evidence for the effects of high CO2 on antipredator behaviour in larval reef fishes. The differences in escape speed between high CO2 and control fish were relatively small; however, when combined with a much longer latency to respond (>30% increase in time to respond) these changes in kinematic responses would likely have a detrimental effect on the probability of a prey fish escaping a predator attack.

Altered behaviour of fish in a high CO2 environment appears to be due to the effects of acid-base regulation on the function of GABA-A receptors (Nilsson et al., 2012; Hamilton, Holcombe & Tresguerres, 2014; Heuer & Grosell, 2014). Fish prevent plasma and tissue acidosis in a high CO2 environment by accumulating HCO 3 in exchange for Cl (Brauner & Baker, 2009). The GABA-A receptor is a gated ion channel that conducts HCO3 and Cl when activated by the neurotransmitter GABA. The altered concentration of HCO 3 and Cl in fish exposed to high CO2 could decrease or reverse the flow of these ions through the GABA-A receptor, causing a reversal in receptor polarization (Heuer & Grosell, 2014). Evidence for the role of GABA-A receptors in behavioural impairment of fish in high CO2 has come from studies showing that treatment with gabazine, a GABA antagonist, reverses the effects of high CO2 on behaviour (Nilsson et al., 2012; Chivers et al., 2014; Chung et al., 2014; Lai, Jutfelt & Nilsson, 2015; Ou et al., 2015; Regan et al., 2016). In the first of these studies, Nilsson et al. (2012) found that gabazine reversed the impaired response to predator odour exhibited by clownfish that had been reared in high CO2. However, in that study gabazine was administered in control seawater and high-CO2 reared fish were tested in control water. Therefore, the effects of gabazine in restoring the response of larval fishes to predator odour has not been previously tested in high-CO2 conditions. Here we show that treatment with 4 mg l−1 gabazine completely reverses the abnormal behavioural of high-CO2 fish to predator odour, restoring their natural aversion to predator odour. This contributes to a large number of independent studies pointing to a key role of GABA-A receptors in the behavioural impairment of fish exposed to high CO2.

While we found no effects of using control or high CO2 water in experimental trials to test the effects of high CO2 on fish behaviour, Sundin & Jutfelt (2016) reported differences in relative lateralization of juvenile goldsinny wrasse when tested in control water versus high CO2 water after 21 days in high CO2. Their study did not include the reverse treatment, where control reared fish were tested in high CO2 water, and there is the potential for learning effects in control fish because the same fish were tested on three different occasions, at days 9, 19 and 21. Nevertheless, that study does suggest that the response of some behavioural traits could be sensitive to differences in the test water used in behavioural trials. Further experiments with a broader range of behavioural traits and using experiments that are not complicated by the repeated testing of the same individuals are required to test this possibility. Furthermore, there are likely to be substantial species-specific differences in the response of fish to control or high CO2 water. For example, Jutfelt & Hedgärde (2013) found that juvenile Atlantic cod were able to distinguish between low and high CO2 water and exhibited a strong preference for low CO2 water. Here, we only tested two species of damselfish, but other species might exhibit different responses. Indeed, there is considerable interspecific variation in the sensitivity of damselfishes to high CO2 (Ferrari et al., 2011a), therefore, it is possible that there may also be species-specific differences in their antipredator responses depending on the test water used in the experimental arena.

Pioneering studies into the effects of high CO2 on fish behaviour used control seawater in experimental trials due to logistical constraints and concerns about the possible effects of low pH on olfactory cues. Our results confirm the results of those initial studies by showing that test water (control of high CO2) had no effect on the antipredator responses exhibited by fish reared in either control or high CO2 conditions. Nevertheless, we do not advocate the continued use of control seawater for testing the behavioural effects of high CO2 in marine organisms. Methods for treating seawater to replicate future pCO2 levels are now sufficiently validated that it is possible to treat most experimental arenas with the respective treatment water that the animals have been reared in, and there may be some behavioural traits that are sensitive to different test water in the experimental trials. The most reliable and unambiguous results will likely come from using the respective treatment water in experimental trials testing the effects of high CO2 on the behaviour of marine organisms. Future studies also need to consider the potential for adaptation of behavioural responses to projected future CO2 levels (Sunday et al., 2014). Individual variation in behavioural tolerance to higher CO2 levels (Munday et al., 2012) could provide the opportunity for selection of more tolerant genotypes and improved performance in a future high CO2 environment.

Supplemental Information

Fig. S1

Experimental apparatus for testing escape responses in juvenile reef fish

DOI: 10.7717/peerj.2501/supp-1

Raw Data for Effects of elevated CO2 on predator avoidance behaviour by reef fishes is not altered by experimental test water

DOI: 10.7717/peerj.2501/supp-2

R Code used in LME analysis of kinematic variables

DOI: 10.7717/peerj.2501/supp-3
35 Citations   Views   Downloads